Prototypes and deflections in
spatial cognition and
Rudolf Laban's choreutics
Jeffrey Scott Longstaff
- jeffrey@laban-analyses.org -
|
An account of one path,
through 20 years of research
Considering CHOREUTICS as:
free lines of motion
(Not linked to fixed points or positions)
continuous process of deflection
(constantly in flux, changing orientation and placement)
Perceptual & Memory Deviations
toward Regular Orientations
Common in Visual, & Kinesthetic Spatial Cognition
1. a Primacy of Dimensional orientations
over Diagonal orientations
2. a Primacy of regular pure 45º Diagonals
over irregular tilting orientations
well known Oblique Effect
Diagonals vs. Dimensionals
1. higher detection thresholds,
2. require longer training periods to learn a selective response,
3. receive slower response,
4. less accurate duplication,
|
|
|
|
faster reaction times |
slower reaction times |
|
|
|
actual path |
recalled closer to right angles |
actual path |
simple, concise, good, regular, logical, coherent, whole
Dimensions = most regular
an 85º angle, perceived as impure right angle (90º)
|
|
|
impure |
PURE |
impure |
dot is placed as if lines are more dimensional
|
place a dot where two lines would intersect |
|
|
ACTUAL orientation |
RECALLED orientation |
Tilted directions recalled closer to Dimensions
|
|
|
|
|
recalled best |
recalled less accurate |
positions learned |
recall after 24 hrs |
|
in a horizontal plane |
in a medial plane |
Memory bias towards pure 45º Diagonal
> Recall location of dot in a circle
ERRORS deviate towards nearest Dimension or pure Diagonal
|
|
|
example image |
recall location of dot? |
example recall deviations |
Subjects spontaneously impose horizontal and vertical boundaries that divide the circle into quadrants. They misplace dots toward a central (prototypic) location in each quadrant (Huttenlocher, Hedges, & Duncan, 1991).
Perception of motion in visual arts
Perceptual deviations influence visual arts
Simplest example; a CIRCLE in a SQUARE
black circle appears to strive or move
toward the closest dimensional or diagonal axis
|
|
|
|
CIRCLE may appear to strive or move in a SQUARE |
|
|
|
most perceived stability |
some perceived motion |
appear to strive |
Structural skeleton |
|
memory schemata
Large number of experiences
perceived & remembered
Small number of regular prototypes (Cognitive Economy)
|
|
(rule of thumb)
perceiving diverse events relative to
a small number of prototypes,
allows quicker perception & reaction time
> Ecologically advantageous
even at the risk of small errors by mis-judging events as more prototypical than Reality
Choreutics prototypes & deflections
> Choreographie (1926) set out in 3D space:
Regular Prototypes Irregular Deflections
3 Dimensions x 8 Diagonals = 24 Inclinations
|
||
3 dimensions |
24 inclinations |
8 diagonals |
PROTOTYPICAL |
||||
1D Dimensions |
1 directional tendency: basic elements | |||
3D Diagonals, pure diagonals | 3 EQUAL directional tendencies | |||
DEFLECTING |
||||
2D Diameters, plane diagonals |
||||
Primary deflected diameters |
2 EQUAL directional tendencies deflected from the dimensions or the diagonals oriented at 45° within a cardinal plane. |
|||
Modified diameters |
2 UN-EQUAL directional tendencies cardinal planes elongate in one dimension, diameters slightly tilt |
|||
3D Inclinations, do not lie in a cardinal plane | ||||
Secondary deflected |
2 EQUAL + 1 UN-EQUAL directional tendencies (1 large + 2 medium) (cube-octahedron) |
|||
Tertiary deflections modified diagonals |
3 UN-EQUAL directional tendencies (1 large, 1 middle, 1 small (icosahedron) |
Choreutic Harmony Theory
> Furthest stage of deflection
Inclinations with 3 un-equal directional tendencies,
most natural or harmonious
Aspects of this Theory, described in many places.
> Dimensions & Diagonals set as spatial contrasts:
The two contrasting fundamentals on which all choreutic harmony is based are the dimensional tension and the diagonal tension. (Laban, 1966, p. 44) |
dimensions, seem to have in themselves certain equilibrating qualities . . . a feeling of stability . . . space diagonals give . . . a feeling of growing disequilibrium, or . . . mobility. (Laban, 1966, p. 90) |
> Organic Harmony as mixtures of contrasts:
PHILOSOPHICALLY:
Since every movement is a composite of stabilising and mobilising tendencies, and since neither pure stability nor pure mobility exist, it will be the deflected or mixed inclinations which are the more apt to reflect trace-forms of living matter. (Laban, 1966, p. 90) |
|
ANATOMICALLY:
. . . the deflected directions are those directions which, in contrast to the stable dimensions and to the labile diagonals, are used by the body most naturally and therefore the most frequently. (Ullmann, 1966, p. 145) Because the body limits the fulfilment of perfect three-dimensional shapes that pure diagonals would offer, most three-dimensional shapes are created through modified diagonals . . . These are available to the body. (Bartenieff & Lewis, 1980, p. 33) |
1D dimensions |
|
|
|
|
|
|
|
|
|
|
Examples of inclinations |
Deflections in Inclination signs
early signs in Choreographie (1926)
Deflecting between 2 contrasting prototypes: Dimension x Diagonal
Diagonals: |
right- |
left- |
left- |
right- |
left- |
right- |
right- |
left- |
||
Dimensions |
|
|
|
|
||||||
vertical |
|
|||||||||
|
|
|
|
|||||||
lateral |
|
|
|
|
|
|||||
sagittal |
|
|
|
|
|
|||||
Inclinations |
READING THE SIGNS: shape = dimension (vertical, lateral, or sagittal)
orientation = the diagonal,
the dot = up or down on that line
Inclination signs in choreutics
Returned to in Choreutics ([1939]1966) as
free space lines & free inclinations
Diagonal signs & Dimensional letter = free inclination
|
and
notation capable of doing this is an old dream in this field of research.
Spatial cognition & choreutics
choreutic concept of deflections
is given validity
by similar structures in spatial cognition studies
both show contrast between:
Prototypes |
Deflections |
|
|
a circle |
variability |
abstract topological form |
variable parameters |
topological forms in continuous fluctuation:
"co-ordinational net of the motor field ...
as oscillating like a cobweb in the wind" (Bernstein, 1984, p. 109)
(idealised prototype) |
(deflections) |
|
|
Topological form triangle 3-ring deflects across polyhedral nets |
Octahedron 3-ring |
Icosahedron 3-ring |
|
|
Hexagonal defence scale deflects to inclinations (icosahedral scaffolding) |
For example: |
|
|
|
|
Twelve-sided
|
|
|
|
transformation of one-dimensional directions into three-dimensional inclinations (Ullmann, 1955, 1971)
Cardinal Planar cycles - Frontal plane - Horizontal plane - Medial plane deflecting into inclinations just one example Cycles can continue to deflect..... |
FRONTAL |
deflecting |
HORIZONTAL |
deflecting |
MEDIAL |
deflecting |
|
|
|
|
|
|
and....
there is no end to this process of deflecting since
the number of possible inclinations is infinite (Laban, 1966, p. 17)
Body control in coordinative structures
Space trajectories must be coordinated in the body
> Mass-spring model of motor control:
limbs as masses
muscles as springs
> Creates oscillatory system, pendulums alternating stretch - recoil automatically responds to unexpected perturbations Biceps as |
|
of muscle collectives basins of attraction in an elastic force field |
|
Center-of-gravity located in the air eg. high-jump center-of-gravity passes under the bar physical body passes over |
|
> Center-of-gravity organises individual parts
individual parts organise as a collective
according to the path of the center
Floating illusion |
|
Passing the center-of-gravity of the arm behind the back eg. A-scale volute hr-bd-lf |
|
Inclination signs lend themselves especially to this mode of control
not fixed at specific spatial points
taken as motion of center-of-gravity of kinematic chain
represent motions of shaping
in a zone linking between body and space
Summary
1. Prototypes & variations identified in perception & memory studies give validity for a similar system of choreutic deflections.
2. Deflecting Inclinations are essential for concepts of harmony in Choreutics
3. Notation signs from Choreographie (1926) make concept of inclinations more explicit
4. Center-of-gravity of kinematic chains can bring Spatial Control into the body
5. Inclination signs are not tied to external grids and so lend themselves to this type of control in a kind of mid-way shaping zone, bridging between body and space
Reference List.
Appelle, S. (1972). Perception and discrimination as a function of stimulus orientation: The oblique effect in man and animals. Psychological Bulletin. 78 (4): 266278.
Arnheim, R. (1974). Art and Visual Perception; The New Version. (expanded and revised) Berkeley: University of California Press. (Originally published 1954).
Attneave, F., & Olson, R. K. (1967). Discriminability of stimuli varying in physical and retinal orientation. Journal of Experimental Psychology. 74: 149-157.
Attneave, F., & Reid, K. W. (1968). Voluntary control of frame of reference and slope equivalence under head rotation. Journal of Experimental Psychology. 78 (1): 153159.
Bartenieff, I., & Lewis, D. (1980). Body Movement; Coping with the Environment. New York: Gordon and Breach.
Bartlett, Sir F. C. (1932). Remembering; A Study in Experimental and Social Psychology. Cambridge: University Press. (Reprinted 1964)
Bernstein, N. (1984). The problem of the interrelation of coordination and localization. In Human Motor Actions; Bernstein Reassessed, edited by H. T. A. Whiting (pp. 77-119). New York: North Holland. (Originally published 1935; Also published in Bernstein, N. [1967] The Co-ordination and Regulation of Movements. Oxford: Pergamon Press. [pp. 15-59].)
Berkinblit, M. B., Feldman, A. G., & Fukson, O. I. (1986). Adaptability of innate motor patterns and motor control mechanisms. Behavioral and Brain Sciences. 9: 585638.
Bizzi, E., and Mussa-Ivaldi, F. A. (1989). Geometrical and mechanical issues in movement planning and control. In Foundations of Cognitive Science, edited by M. I. Posner (pp. 769-792). Cambridge Massachusetts: M. I. T. Press.
Bizzi, E., Chapple, W., and Hogan, N. (1982). Mechanical properties of muscles; Implications for motor control. Trends in Neurosciences. 5: 395-398.
Byrne, R. W. (1979). Memory for Urban Geography. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology. 31: 147-154.
Clark, F. J., and Burgess, P. R. (1984). Longterm memory for position. Unpublished manuscript, Dept. of Physiology and Biophysics, University of Nebraska College of Medicine: Omaha. (Reviewed in Clark and Horch 1986, p. 26).
Clark, F. J., and Horch, K. W. (1986). Kinesthesia. In Handbook of Perception and Human Performance; Volume 1, Sensory Processes and Perception, edited by K. R. Boff, L. Kaufmann, and J. P. Thomas (pp. 13.1-13.62). New York: John Wiley and Sons.
Cummings, J. F. and Caprarola, M. A. (1986). Schmidts schema theory: Variability of practice and transfer. Journal of Human Movement Studies. 12: 51-57.
Dell, C. (1972). Space Harmony Basic Terms. Revised by A. Crow (1969). Revised by I. Bartenieff (1977). Dance Notation Bureau: New York. (First published 1966; Fourth printing 1979)
Easton, T. A. (1972). On the Normal use of reflexes. American Scientist. 60: 591-599.
Easton, T. A. (1978). Coordinative structures - The basis for a motor program. In Psychology of Motor Behavior and Sport - 1977, edited by D. M. Landers and R. W. Christina (pp. 63-81). Champaigh, Illinois: Human Kinetics.
Evarts, E. V. and Tanji, J. (1974). Gating of motor cortex reflexes by prior instruction. Brain Research. 71: 479-494.
Fitch, H.L., Tuller, B. and Turvey, M. T. (1982). The Bernstein perspective: III. Tuning of coordinative structures with special reference to perception. In Human Motor Behavior, edited by J. A. S. Kelso (pp. 271-281). New Jersey: Lawrence Erlbaum.
Gurfinkel, V. S., Kots, Ya. M., Krinskiy, V. I., Paltsev, E. I., Feldman, A. G., Tsetlin, M. L., & Shik, M. L. (1971). Concerning tuning before movement. In Models of the Structural-Functional Organization of Certain Biological Systems, edited by I. M. Gelfand, V. S. Gurfinkel, S. V. Fomin and M. L. Tsetlin (pp. 361-372). Translated by C. R. Beard. Cambridge: M. I. T. Press.
Hackney, P. (1998). Making Connections - Total Body Integration through Bartenieff Fundamentals. Amsterdam: Gordon and Breach.
Hutchinson, A. (1970). Labanotation or Kinetography Laban: The System of Analyzing and Recording Movement (3rd revised edition 1977). New York: Theatre Arts Books. (First published 1954)
Huttenlocher, J., Hedges, L. V., and Duncan, S. (1991). Categories and particulars: Prototype effects in estimating spatial location. Psychological Review. 98 (3): 352-376.
Jordan, M. I., and Rosenbaum, D. A. (1989). Action. In Foundations of Cognitive Science, edited by M. I. Posner (pp. 727-767). Cambridge Massachusetts: M. I. T. Press.
Kelso, J. A. S. & Holt, K. G. (1980). Exploring a vibratory systems analysis of human movement production. Journal of Neurophysiology. 43 (5): 1183-1196.
Koffka, K. (1935). Principles of Gestalt Psychology. London: Routledge and Kegan Paul. (4th printing 1955)
Kugler, P. N., Kelso, J. A. S., & Turvey, M. T. (1982). On the control and coordination of naturally developing systems. In The Development of Movement Control and Co-ordination, edited by J. A. S. Kelso and J. E. Clark (pp. 5-78). John Wiley & Sons.
Laban, R. (1926). Choreographie (German). Jena: Eugen Diederichs. (Unpublished English translation, edited by J. S. Longstaff, 2000; London: Laban Collection archives)
Laban, R. (1928). Schrifttanz; Methodik, Orthographie, Erläuterungen [Written dance; Methodology, Orthography, Explanations] (German). Vienna: Universal Edition. (English and French editions published 1930)
Laban, R. (1966 [1939]). Choreutics (annotated and edited by L. Ullmann). London: MacDonald and Evans. (Published in USA as The language of movement: a guide book to choreutics Boston: Plays)
Laban, R. (1984). A Vision of Dynamic Space (Compiled by L. Ullmann). London: Falmer Press.
Laws, K. 1984. The physics of dance. New York: Schirmer.
Longstaff, J. S. (1987). The theory and practice of volute spatial forms. Certification Project in Laban Movement Analysis, Laban / Bartenieff Institute of Movement Studies, New York.
Longstaff, J. S. (1989). Moving in Crystals: A continued Integration of Polyhedral Geometry with Rudolf Labans Choreutics towards its use as a Choreographic Tool. Eugene, Oregon: Microform Publications, College of Human Development and Performance. (M.S. Thesis, University of Oregon, 1988)
Longstaff, J. S. (1996). Cognitive structures of kinesthetic space; Reevaluating Rudolf Labans choreutics in the context of spatial cognition and motor control. Ph.D. Thesis. London: City University, Laban Centre.
Longstaff, J. S. (1998). Subjective organisation in the recall of abstract body movements. Perceptual and Motor Skills, 86, 931-940.
Longstaff, J. S. (Ed.) (2000a). Rudolf Labans Choreographie. Unpublished English translation and annotated edition of Choreographie (Laban, 1926). (London: Laban Collection Archives)
Longstaff, J. S. (2000b). Re-evaluating Rudolf Labans Choreutics. Perceptual and Motor Skills, 91, 191-210.
Longstaff, J. S. (2000c). Organisations of Chaos in an Infinitely Deflecting Body Space. Paper presented at A Conference on Liminality and Performance, Brunel University Twickenham; 27-30 April.
Longstaff, J. S. (2001a). A free space vector approach to Labans choreutics. Movement and Dance Magazine of the Laban Guild, 20 (2, Summer), 10-11. (Invited)
Longstaff, J. S. (2001b). Translating vector symbols from Labans (1926) Choreographie. In Proceedings of the Twenty-second Biennial Conference of the International Council of Kinetography Laban, 26 July - 2 August (pp. 70-76). Ohio State University, Columbus, Ohio. USA: ICKL.
Longstaff, J. S. (2002). Choreutic Tetrahedra. Movement News (Laban / Bartenieff Institute of Movement Studies), 27 (1), 10-12.
Longstaff, J. S. (2003). A model for practical kinesthesia. Poster presentation at the 13th Annual Conference of the International Association for Dance Medicine & Science (IADMS). 24-26 October. LABAN, London.
Longstaff, J. S. (2004). Symmetries in the minuet from Laban's (1926) Choreographie. In Proceedings of the twenty-third biennial conference of the International Council of Kinetography Laban (ICKL), 23 - 28 July (pp. 174-179). Beijing Normal University, China:ICKL.
Longstaff, J. S. (2005). Rudolf Labans notation workbook, an historical survey of dance script methods from Choreographie (1926). In Proceedings of the twenty-fourth biennial conference of the International Council of Kinetography Laban (ICKL), 29 July - 4 August. LABAN centre, London:ICKL. (in press)
Maffei, L., & Campbell, F. W. (1970). Neurophysiological localization of the vertical and horizontal visual coordinates in man. Science. 167 (Jan.): 386-387.
Matin, E., & Drivas, A. (1979). Acuity for orientation measured with a sequential recognition task and signal detection methods. Perception & Psychophysics. 25 (3): 161-168.
Moar, I. (1978). Mental triangulation and the nature of internal representations of space. Ph.D. diss. Cambridge: St. Johns College.
Moar, I., & Bower, G. H. (1983). Inconsistency in spatial knowledge. Memory & Cognition. 11 (2): 107-113.
Mussa-Ivaldi, F. A., Hogan, N., and Bizzi, E. (1985). Neural, mechanical, and geometric factors subserving arm posture in humans. The Journal of Neuroscience. 5 (10): 2732-2743.
Pew, R. W., & Rosenbaum, D. A. (1988). Human movement control: Computation, representation, and implementation. In Stevens Handbook of Experimental Psychology; Vol. 2. Learning and Cognition, 2nd edition edited by R. C. Atkinson, R. J. Herrnstein, G. Lindzey, and R. D. Luce (pp. 473-509). New York: John Wiley.
Pollick, F. E., & Ishimura, G. (1996) The three-dimensional curvature of straight-ahead movements. Journal of Motor Behavior, 28 (3), 271279.
Rosch, E. (1973). Natural Categories. Cognitive Psychology. 4: 328-350.
Rosch, E. (1975). Cognitive reference points. Cognitive Psychology. 7: 532-547.
Sadalla, E. K., Burroughs, W. J., and Staplin, L. J. (1980). Reference points in spatial cognition. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Learning and Memory. 6 (5): 516-528.
Schmidt, R. A. (1975). A schema theory of discrete motor skilllearning. Psychological Review. 82 (4): 225-260.
Schmidt, R. A. (1976). The schema as a solution to some persistent proplems in motor learning theory. In Motor Control; Issues and Trends, edited by G. E. Stelmach (pp. 41-65). New York: Academic Press.
Schmidt, R. A. (1982). Motor Control and Learning a Behavioral Emphasis. Champaign Illinois: Human Kinetics.
Tuller, B., Turvey, M. T., and Fitch, H. L. (1982). The Bernstein perspective: II. The concept of muscle linkage or coordinative structure. In Human Motor Behavior, edited by J. A. S. Kelso (pp. 253-270). New Jersey: Lawrence Erlbaum.
Tversky, B. (1981). Distortions in memory for maps. Cognitive Psychology. 13: 407-433.
Ullmann, L. (1955). Space Harmony - VI. Laban Art of Movement Guild Magazine, 15 (Oct.), 2934.
Ullmann, L. (1966). Rudiments of space-movement. In R. Laban, Choreutics (annotated and edited by L. Ullmann) (pp. 138-210). London: MacDonald and Evans.
Ullmann, L. (1971). Some Preparatory Stages for the Study of Space Harmony in Art of Movement. Surrey: Laban Art of Movement Guild.
Weintraub, D. J., & Virsu, V. (1971). The misperception of angles: Estimating the vertex of converging line segments. Perception & Psychophysics. 9 (1A): 5-8.
Weintraub, D. J., & Virsu, V. (1972). Extimating the vertex of converging lines: Angle misperception? Perception & Psychophysics. 11 (4): 277-283.
Wertheimer, M. (1923). Laws of organization in perceptual forms. Reprinted in A Source Book of Gestalt Psychology, (4th edition 1969) translated and edited by W. D. Ellis (pp. 71-88). London: Routledge and Kegan Paul.
Wyke, M. (1965). Comparative analysis of proprioception in left and right arms. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology. 17: 149-157.